Motion Stereopsis (Binocular Depth-from-Motion, BDFM)

[ as a counterpart, the conventional stereopsis is Disparity Stereopsis (Binocular Depth-from-Disparity, BDFD) ]

 

Ai-Hou Wang, MD. PhD.

 

 

(Some images in this article require red-blue glasses. Right eye blue lens, left eye red lens.)

 

This article partly originates from the 1999 Arthur Jampolsky fellows' meeting

Proceedings of the Jampolsky Festschrift, The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute (SKERI), San Francisco, April 2000

 

Since Sir Charles Wheatstone (1802–1887), https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Charles_Wheatstone, invented the stereoscope (see figure), stereoscopic vision has been attributed to the disparity between the images seen by the two eyes - the left and right eyes are like two cameras or video cameras, and the images seen are slightly dissimilar. The brain can convert these slightly dissimilar parts into depth perception. This depth perception derived from disparity is called stereoscopic vision or stereopsis.

 

 

Conversely, simulating the images seen by both eyes, placing two slightly dissimilar images (see figure) on Wheatstone's stereoscope for the left and right eyes can also allow the brain to transform into depth perception. As shown in the figure, the cube and double circles are simulations of real three-dimensional objects.

 

     

 

Some people look at the images cross-eyed – the image for the right eye is placed on the left, and the image for the left eye is placed on the right. This makes it easier to merge the images from both eyes, easier to see a three-dimensional image with depth perception.

 

   

 

In the image, the blue square appears slightly to the left in the right eye's view and slightly to the right in the left eye's view. Stereoscopic vision in the brain translates this disparity into the blue square appearing to float above the yellow background.

 

  

 

In 1960, Bela Julesz at Bell Labs invented and created a random-dot stereogram (RDS) based on the same principle (see figure). The hidden shaped within random-dot stereograms cannot be seen with either eye alone; they are only visible after the texture of random dots are fused by both eyes. The principle behind creating random-dot stereograms is the same as that of contour stereograms – the square in the right eye is slightly shifted to the left, and the square in the left eye is slightly shifted to the right. This remains utilizing the "disparity" between the images seen by both eyes to create depth perception.

 

 

 

Some people find it easier to see the floating square by crossing their eyes – placing the image for the right eye on the left side, and the image for the left eye on the right side.

 

 

This article introduces another binocular mechanism for depth perception besides disparity – Motion stereopsis, or Binocular Depth-from-Motion, BDFM.

 

When we observe an object moving in different directions in space, it will create different combinations of two motion vectors on the retinas of both eyes (see figure). Conversely, different combinations of two motion vectors on the retinas of both eyes contain sufficient information to calculate and to perceive its direction of motion in depth. This is motion stereopsis. It needs to be tested separately from the stereopsis coming from binocular disparity that we are familiar with.

 

 

Motion in the left-right directions results in identical-magnitude, same-direction motion vectors on the retinas of both eyes (Figures 1 and 7); motion in the front-back directions results in opposite-direction, identical-magnitude motion vectors on the retinas of both eyes (Figures 4 and 10) (see figure).

 

 

Disparity stereopsis is a static depth perception (Depth-from-Disparity) arising from the dissimilarity in retinal images of the two eyes; while motion stereopsis is dynamic, arising from the difference in motion vectors of the two eyes' retinal images (Depth-from-Motion). In Wheatstone's era, perhaps due to the lack of dynamic displays of modern computer, another mechanism of binocular depth perception, other than disparity, arising from motion perception was not recognized.

 

Static stereopsis is the depth perception generated by the disparity between the two semi-stereo images of the left and right eye. Dynamic stereopsis is not simply a dynamic version of disparity stereopsis; some papers refer to it as "dynamic stereopsis", which is inappropriate, failing to grasp the key to the physiological mechanism of motion stereopsis. Motion stereopsis is the depth perception generated by the difference in "motion perception" between the two eyes.

 

ayama of Harvard University's Department of Psychology argued that motion perception should be studied and discussed independently of form (contour) perception. Similarly, "motion stereopsis" should also be discussed separately from "disparity stereopsis," as a parallel topic. This article describes testing disparity stereopsis and motion stereopsis using a random-dot dynamic stereogram, allowing for separate testing of depth perception derived from disparity cues and depth perception derived from motion cues.

 

Nakayama K. Biological image motion processing: a review. Vision Res. 1985;25(5):625-60.

 

An important point to note is that motion perception is a sensory (afference) rather than a motor (efference) event. Motion vs. Motor can very often be confusing.

 

Search YouTube for "Motion Stereopsis" for 10-minute explanatory videos on motion stereopsis.

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=j9PaE92JBKk&t=5s (Chinese)

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=aEoxO_RdGhE&t=17s (English)

 

 

Origin

 

Dr. Ji-Hau Jan, my deceased colleague, was adept at computer programming. He designed a computer version of NTU RDS (National Taiwan University, Random Dot Stereograms) for Dr. Luke Lin and Health Promotion Administration, Ministry of Health and Welfare for vision screening of preschool and school children.

The title page of this program exhibited a stereogram with a square changing its depth repeatedly. The disparity varied from 1 to 31 and then 31 to 1 pixel(s) (see figure). This created a dynamic stereogram, similar to an animated motion picture, showing a floating square whose depth oscillated continuously between two depths.

 

    

 

This dynamic title page inadvertently touches upon another topic in the visual study of random dot patterns – the random-dot kinematogram (RDK) or the random-dot cinematogram (RDC). An RDK refers to that either left eye or right eye views this title page to be a dynamic random dot pattern. No square is visible at any single moment. However, when the animation starts playing, a square is seen moving leftward and rightward. RDK is a monocular visual phenomenon, a topic related to temporal form perception.

 

A person without stereopsis watching this dynamic title page found that he could perceive the depth. Contrary to RDS where being able to see the hidden shape serves as evidence of stereopsis, the square is not hidden in the RDK. Being able to see the square in RDK does not prove that the subject has depth perception. However, reporting Emmert's Law does indicate depth perception. Emmert’s Law says that when the square appears to float and get closer to you, the square appears smaller. In the real world, when a fixed-size square moves closer to you, the retinal image gets larger, but you interpret the square as fixed-size. The square in the RDK has fixed-size; when it floats and gets closer, its retinal image does not get bigger, so you conversely perceive it as getting smaller. Experiencing Emmert's Law, this person without stereopsis should genuinely be experiencing depth perception.

 

If the RDK on the title page is paused, it becomes a single, static RDS. Persons with normal stereopsis will see the forward and backward moving square suddenly stop, remaining suspended in midair; while persons without stereopsis will find the square disappears to be invisible.

 

This implies that, in addition to disparity cue, motion cue can also generate depth perception.

 

 

We employed 4-alternative-forced-choice (4-AFC) paradigm, displaying four moving squares on the screen. One square moves forward and backward in depth, while the other three move left and right on a plane. Subjects are required to find the square that is moving in depth (see figure).

 

 

Based on the principle of motion stereopsis, for the square moving in depth, the squares viewed by right and left eye move in opposite direction, while for those squares moving on a plane, the squares viewed by right and left eye move along the same direction (see figure).

 

  

 

  

 

We create dynamic stereograms with random dot patterns. Please notice only the 3D square jumping in depth, of which

the squares viewed by right and left eye move in opposite direction.

 

From disparity cue, at time 1, it’s a 3D square coming in front; at time 2, it’s a 3D square recessed back.

Viewing with the right eye, it’s an RDK with motion cue; viewing with the left eye, it’s also an RDK with motion cue, yet moving in opposite direction.

When viewing with both eyes, you see a square moving back and forth in depth. The depth perception comes from both disparity and motion cues (see figure).

 

  

 

If the squares at time 1 and time 2 are different random dot patterns – at time 1, it’s a 3D square coming in front from disparity cue; at time 2, it’s a 3D square recessed back, also from disparity cue.

Viewing with the right eye, it is a random-dot correlogram (RDC) at time-domain (or temporal RDC); viewing with the left eye, it is also a temporal RDC.  There is no left-right movement of square in either eye.

When viewing with both eyes, a square appears to jump back and forth in depth, which is entirely from disparity cue, and there is no motion cue (see figure).

 

See RDCorrelogram for the explanation of random-dot correlogram (RDC).

 

  

 

If the squares in the left and right eyes have different random dot patterns – at time 1, it is a spatial RDC; at time 2, it is also a spatial RDC. Pause the dynamic random-dot pattern at time 1 or time 2 shows no disparity between the random-dot pattern of the right eye and the left eye, and thus no depth perception.

Viewing with the right eye, it’s an RDK; viewing with the left eye only, it’s also an RDK, with the square moves in the opposite direction to the right eye.

When viewing with both eyes, a square appears to jump back and forth in depth. This depth perception is purely from motion cue without disparity cue intervenes (see figure).

 

  

 

It is summarized in the following figure,

 

 

 

For stereogram with disparity cue only, the squares at time 1 and time 2 are different random dot patterns, which constitute temporal RDC. For stereogram with motion cue only, the squares for the left eye and right eye are different random dot patterns, which constitute spatial RDC.

In fact, two random-dot patterns may not only be completely identical or completely different. In between these extreme conditions, two random-dot patterns can be partially identical. From completely identical to completely different, it can be modulated gradually.

 

Any two random-dot patterns with 50% density, when looking at each pixel's position, consists of 1/4 positions where the pixels in pattern 1 and pattern 2 are both black; 1/4 positions where the pixels in pattern 1 and pattern 2 are both white; 1/4 positions where the pixels in pattern 1 are white while the pixels in pattern 2 are black and 1/4 positions where the pixels in pattern 1 are black while the pixels in pattern 2 are white. In other words, at half positions the pixels in pattern 1 and pattern 2 are identical and at another half positions the pixels in pattern 1 and pattern 2 are at opposite polarity.

 

Conversely, if you change the polarity of half (50%) pixels in a 50% density random-dot pattern, i.e. changing black into white and white into black, it becomes a completely different 50% density random-dot pattern. The logic operation XOR is the exact operator to change the polarity of pixels. An operator of 50% density will convert one 50% density random-dot pattern into another completely different 50% density random-dot pattern (see figure).

 

 

If the operator is of 20% density, it changes the polarity of 20% pixels of the original random-dot pattern to convert it into another 50% density random-dot pattern. The new random-dot pattern is partially different from and partially identical to the original random-dot pattern. It will keep some ghost image of the original random-dot pattern (see figure)!

 

 

If the operator is of 100% density, it inverts the polarity of all pixels of the original random-dot pattern and converts it into the negative image of the original random-dot pattern. The resultant random-dot pattern nevertheless remains to be of 50% density (see image).

 

 

In generating motion stereogram with disparity cue only, the random-dot pattern of the square at time 1 and time 2 are different. Here, we can apply a temporal operator (temp) of different densities to modulate the resultant random-dot pattern from exactly identical to completely different.

Similarly, in generating motion stereogram with motion cue only, the random-dot pattern of the square for the left and right eye are different. We can apply spatial operators (spat) of different densities to modulate the random-dot pattern from exactly identical to completely different (see figure).

 

 

If you are interested in visual science, you can titrate the spatial operator from 0% to 50%. It will change the disparity cue from all to none.

You can also titrate the temporal operator from 0% to 50%. It will on the other hand change the motion cue from all to none.

You may observe how different people's depth perception relies on different proportions of disparity cue and motion cue (see figure).

 

 

For simplicity in the clinical work, we tested only (1) depth from full disparity and full motion cues (Spat=0%, Temp=0%), (2) depth from full disparity cue with zero motion cue (Spat=0%, Temp=50%) and (3) depth from full motion cue with zero disparity cue (Spat=50%, Temp=0%).

If both disparity and motion cues are removed by using 50% spatial operator and 50% temporal operator, it will surely be impossible to get any depth perception.

 

 

Motion stereopsis is a dynamic binocular visual function. Clinically, dynamic tests are rare, perhaps due to technological limitations. Currently, binocular visual function tests mainly fusion and static stereopsis. About their mechanism, motor fusion can be interpreted as Vergence-from-Disparity, and stereopsis as Depth-from-Disparity. Both are binocular consequences to the static environment objects.

 

We live in a dynamic world. The retinal images are mostly in motion. The depth perception coming from comparison of the motion vectors of the retinal images of two eyes might be used even more frequently. Surprisingly, we don’t have corresponding clinical examinations for that!

 

Kitaoji and Toyama called this mechanism ‘motion stereopsis’. I prefer to name it as ‘Binocular Depth-from-Motion, BDFM’ according more exactly to its physiological mechanism. At the same time I would interpret the conventional stereopsis as ‘Binocular Depth-from-Disparity’ or ‘Disparity stereopsis’.

 

Kitaoji H, Toyama K. Preservation of position and motion stereopsis in strabismic subjects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1987;28:1260-1267.

 

Regan provided a thorough description of ‘the depth perception from object motion’ and ‘the spatial motion of an object’. That is Depth-from-Motion and Motion-in-Depth respectively.

 

Regan D. Depth from motion and motion-in-depth. In: Cronly-Dillon JR, ed. Vision and visual dysfunction, Vol. 9. Houndmills: Macmillan. - Regan D, ed. Binocular vision. Chapter 8. 1991;137-169.

 

The study of spatial motion is twofold – 1. ‘looming’ or ‘optic flow’ describes and locates the movement of ourselves in space, 2. depth-from-motion describes the movement of objects in space.

The spatial movement of objects produces different combinations of motion vectors on the two retinas, and conversely, from these different combinations of vectors, we have sufficient information to calculate and infer the direction of movement of the object in space.

Note that the terminology ‘motion’ in depth-from-motion refers to the image motion on the retina. It’s a sensory perception rather than motor event. While the ‘motion’ in motion-in-depth refers to the movement of the object in space.

The image motion on the retina of a single eye also evokes certain perceptions of distance and depth. That’s why I emphasize ‘Binocular’ Depth-from-Motion as the study object of this article.

 

 

Motion stereopsis or BDFM of strabismic patients

 

We have tested 54 patients with small-angle strabismus, some post-operatively and some did not receive any surgery.

 These subjects failed the 35 min-of-arc static random-dot stereopsis test (NTU RDS 35min) yet passed the motion stereopsis test. They passed the RDS test with both disparity and motion cues, passed the RDS test with motion cue only, but failed the RDS test with disparity cue only (see figure).

 

 

This demonstrates that their depth perception comes from motion cue. They cannot pass this stereopsis test with disparity cue only, which is consistent with their inability to pass static random-dot stereograms, such as TNO stereopsis test.

 

Individuals who fail conventional stereopsis tests lack the binocular disparity mechanism of stereopsis. In daily life, motion cues serves as their binocular mechanism for judging distance. Many strabismic patients who fail conventional stereopsis tests report that they still perceive depth and distance when watching 3D movies.

 

Another example is Pulfrich pendulum – viewing a pendulum with one eye through a neutral density filter will produce Pulfrich phenomenon. The pendulum moving in a single plane appears to rotate along an elliptical trajectory, moving closer to and farther from the observer (see figure).

 

 

The Pulfrich phenomenon is generally explained with static disparity mechanisms, stating that the neutral density filter over one eye causes neural conduction delay. This delay in time creates disparity between the two eyes, thus generating depth perception. In fact, binocular motion perception mechanisms also participate in this process, which can be demonstrated using random-dot correlograms (RDC), removing the mechanism of disparity.

 

 

Among 54 patients, 9 had early-onset strabismus, as their optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) showed naso-temporal asymmetry and/or coexistent latent nystagmus. Although early surgical intervention for infantile esotropia often fails to restore their disparity stereopsis, motion stereopsis can be restored when the strabismus is corrected to certain smaller angle. Consequently, early surgery for infantile esotropia, beyond the cosmesis, also has functional benefits of binocular vision and the ability to enjoy 3D movies.

 

 

Motion Stereopsis or BDFM in subjects with normal stereopsis

 

We have tested subjects with normal disparity stereopsis. They often find that the stereograms with motion cue only are harder to read than those with disparity cue only. Yet with practice, most can pass the motion stereopsis test with motion cue only. There’re nevertheless two persons who remained not being able to pass, no matter how hard they had practiced!

 

We interpret as that people with normal stereopsis seem accustomed to using disparity mechanisms to judge distance, putting aside the motion mechanism, although the motion mechanism does exist in their brains.

 

 

Neurophysiology of Binocular Depth-from-Motion (BDFM)

 

In everyday life, objects with motion-in-depth, such as a mosquito in flight, their images on two eyes typically have two characteristics – changing disparity and changing motion vectors. Cumming and Parker investigated two possible binocular mechanisms for detecting moving objects in depth. One is to detect changes of motion vectors between the two eyes (depth-from-motion); the other is to detect changes of image disparities over time (depth-from-disparity) (see figure).

 

Cumming BG, Parker AJ. Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth. Vis Res 1994;34:483-495.

 

 

They believe it is the second mechanism, arguing that there is no evidence to support the human body's ability to detect the difference between motion vectors of the two eyes. Our clinical observations, however, tend to support the first mechanism, at least in patients with strabismus. Because strabismus patients lack the ability to detect disparity, they obviously cannot calculate the change of disparity over time.

 

 

The stereopsis examination we conduct in the strabismus/amblyopia clinic can be simplified into three steps:

 

Step 1: 300 seconds-of-arc random dot stereogram (NTU RDS 300 sec-of-arc)

(The visual screening of students is also based on NTU RDS 300 sec-of-arc as the criterion for passing/failing.)

If this test is passed, it is accepted as normal stereopsis. If not pass, we proceed to the next step

 

Step 2: 35 minutes-of-arc (=2100 seconds-of-arc) random dot stereogram (NTU RDS 35 min-of-arc) (2100 sec-of-arc)

If this test is passed, it is accepted as subnormal stereopsis. Patients with esophoria and anisometropia often fall into this category. Their stereopsis is not perfect yet is not entirely absent.

We explain to the parents that being able to pass the coarse random dot stereopsis test is already a fairly good binocular vision function. If still not pass, we proceed to the next step

 

Step 3: Motion stereopsis test

If this test is passed, we will explain to the parents that although they cannot read static stereograms on the books, they will be able to watch dynamic 3D movies (motion pictures / animations).

 

If they fail again, we explain to the parents that not having stereopsis does not cause much inconvenience in daily life. Monocular cue also gives spatial perception and distance judgment.

 

The computer section of Universal Eye Center has made a cross-platform program of stereopsis examination for public use, which includes both disparity and motion stereopsis.

 

https://www.eyedoctor.com.tw/Stereopsis/

一張含有 文字, 螢幕擷取畫面, 摩天大樓, 建築 的圖片

AI 產生的內容可能不正確。

一張含有 文字, 螢幕擷取畫面, 眼鏡, 太陽眼鏡 的圖片

AI 產生的內容可能不正確。

 

By the way, you may also be interested in the stereopsis SCREENING program we made:

 

https://www.eyedoctor.com.tw/Stereopsis_Screening/

一張含有 文字, 螢幕擷取畫面, 海報 的圖片

AI 產生的內容可能不正確。

一張含有 文字, 螢幕擷取畫面, 眼鏡, 太陽眼鏡 的圖片

AI 產生的內容可能不正確。

 

 

Professor Takashi Fujikado (ふじかど たかし) of the Department of Medical Research, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University, is also interested in motion stereopsis. He once gave a lecture at a pediatric ophthalmology/strabismus conference in mainland China. The topic is "Can people without stereopsis watch 3D movies?". The subject must be the same as the main theme of this article - motion stereopsis!

 

 

Clinical studies at Nagoya University suggest a relationship between sensory fusion (as assessed by the Worth-4-dots test) and the presence of motion stereopsis.

 

Maeda M, Sato M, Ohmura T, Miyazaki Y, Wang AH, Awaya S. Binocular depth-from-motion in infantile and late-onset esotropia patients with poor stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1999;40:3031-3036.

 

Clinical examination of binocular vision generally considers motor fusion a prerequisite for traditional stereopsis (disparity stereopsis). Is motion stereopsis also a prerequisite for disparity stereopsis? Is motion stereopsis a more fundamental and basic form of binocular vision than disparity stereopsis? Or are they parallel, independent entities of binocular depth perception? (See figure)

 

 

In my clinical practice, I test disparity random-dot stereopsis first; if it fails, I test next the motion stereopsis. Many people failed the disparity stereopsis but could pass the motion stereopsis test.

 

So, are there people with normal disparity stereopsis but no motion stereopsis? Laby found that some people with normal stereopsis lack dynamic stereopsis. He therefore considered disparity stereopsis and motion stereopsis as two parallel mechanisms.

 

Laby DM, Kirschen DG. Dynamic stereoacuity: priliminary results and normative data for a new test for the quantitative measurement of motion in depth. Binocular Vis & Eye Muscle Surgery. 1995;10:191-200.

 

We have two patients with normal stereopsis, who, even after practice, could not pass the motion stereopsis test with motion cue only. The neural mechanisms relating motion stereopsis and disparity stereopsis require further study and discussion.

 

e patient had residual esotropia of 18Δ – she had 27Δ esotropia and wore 9Δ base-out prism glasses. Among our series of patients, she is the one with largest deviation angle who passed the motion stereopsis test. This suggests that the receptive field of the motion stereopsis neural mechanism is greater than 18Δ.

 

 

Infantile esotropia (or Congenital esotropia)

 

For infantile esotropia, the general consensus is surgery before the age of two. Despite it is impossible to regain random dot stereopsis, monofixation with peripheral fusion and motion stereopsis can be restored.

 

The disadvantage of early surgery is the higher chance of reoperation. Doctors who emphasize the early surgery are focused on the restoration of binocular vision. It is generally believed that the earlier the surgery, the greater the chance to restore motor fusion. But whether motion stereopsis necessarily needs such early surgery for restoration, or whether it can be restored with later surgery, requires further clinical experience to answer this question.

 

 

Some people cite the motion stereopsis test procedure but change the name to ‘dynamic stereopsis’ which loses the fundamental concept that this stereopsis is based on the difference of motion perception of two eyes. Motion stereopsis (Binocular Depth-from-Motion) is another mechanism to generate binocular depth perception, as opposed to disparity stereopsis (Binocular Depth-from-Disparity).

 

Static stereopsis is the depth perception generated from the ‘disparity’ between the two half-stereograms viewed by left and right eyes. Motion stereopsis is not a dynamic version of disparity stereopsis; some papers call it ‘dynamic stereopsis’ which is inappropriate and fails to grasp the key points of the physiological mechanisms of motion stereopsis. Motion stereopsis is the depth perception generated from the difference of ‘motion perception’ between the two eyes.

 

Motion stereopsis originates from motion perception, and it is, of course, a dynamic testing method. However, the term ‘dynamic stereopsis’ has other applications. The figure below depicts the pattern designed to record the visual evoked potential (VEP) of stereopsis, consisting of 16 stereograms of hidden checkerboards. Eight stereograms comprise checkerboards with a raised checker at its upper left corner, and eight stereograms comprise checkerboards with a recessed checker at its upper left corner. They are played repeatedly, mimicking the checkerboard reversal patterns used in clinical VEP examinations. However, this is not a black/white reversal pattern, but a depth reversal pattern. The images seen by the left eye and right eye individually seem like a snowstorm pattern after the television ceases broadcasting. When viewed with both eyes, the checkerboard undergoes depth reversal at a preset frequency. If after signal analysis the visual evoked potential contains this particular frequency, it indicates a brain response induced by stereopsis, as there is no such frequency input from each eye individually.

 

This figure is what previous researchers called a dynamic random-dot stereogram. It’s not the motion stereogram as we discuss in this article. The background and foreground of each stereogram are all different, and do not contain motion cues. The depth perception comes from the disparity between the half-stereograms viewed by left and right eyes, which is different from the motion stereopsis mentioned in this article – despite both are ‘dynamic’ forms of stereograms.

 

 

 

References

l  Kitaoji H, Toyama K. Preservation of position and motion stereopsis in strabismic subjects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1987;28:1260-1267.

l  Laby DM, Kirschen DG. Dynamic stereoacuity: priliminary results and normative data for a new test for the quantitative measurement of motion in depth. Binocular Vis & Eye Muscle Surgery. 1995;10:191-200.

l  Regan D. Depth from motion and motion-in-depth. In: Cronly-Dillon JR, ed. Vision and visual dysfunction, Vol. 9. Houndmills: Macmillan. - Regan D, ed. Binocular vision. Chapter 8. 1991;137-169.

l  Nakayama K. Biological image motion processing: a review. Vision Res. 1985;25(5):625-60.

l  Cumming BG, Parker AJ. Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth. Vis Res 1994;34:483-495.

l  Maeda M, Sato M, Ohmura T, Miyazaki Y, Wang AH, Awaya S. Binocular depth-from-motion in infantile and late-onset esotropia patients with poor stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1999;40:3031-3036.

l  Wang AH. Proceedings of the Jampolsky Festschrift, The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute (SKERI), San Francisco, April 2000.

 

 

BDFM – Both Motion and Disparity cues

 

BDFM – Both Motion and Disparity cues – Right one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Both Motion and Disparity cues – Upper one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Both Motion and Disparity cues – Lower one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Both Motion and Disparity cues – Left one is jumping in depth

 

 

BDFM - Disparity cue only

 

BDFM - Disparity cue only – Lower one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Disparity cue only – Right one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Disparity cue only – Left one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Disparity cue only – Upper one is jumping in depth

 

 

BDFM - Motion cue only

 

BDFM - Motion cue only – Left one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Motion cue only – Upper one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Motion cue only – Right one is jumping in depth

 

BDFM - Motion cue only – Lower one is jumping in depth

 

 

Another thing that is related to motion stereopsis / binocular depth-from-motion is the Pulfrich pendulum (Pulfrich effect)

 

If one of the eyes is looking through a neutral density filter, or there is optic neuritis in one eye, the pendulum swinging on a frontal plane without depth will appear to move along an elliptical orbit (as shown in the figure), resulting in binocular depth perception in both eyes.

 

 

The general explanation is that the pendulum seen by the eye with light reduction or optic neuritis creates a delay. At any moment the pendulum seen by this eye seems to be located at an earlier position than the other eye. And there appears a disparity of the pendulums seen by the two eyes.

 

If the delay is in the left eye, when the pendulum swings to the right, the pendulum seen by the left eye is more left, and the pendulum seen by the right eye is more right, resulting in uncrossed disparity, so it feels farther away from us; When the pendulum swings to the left, the pendulum seen by the left eye is more to the right, and the pendulum seen by the right eye is more to the left, resulting in crossed disparity, so it feels closer to us.

The pendulum thus seems to go along an elliptical trajectory with depth. Viewed from above, the trajectory rotates in a clockwise (CW) direction. If the delay is in the right eye, by the same reasoning, it will be seen as going in a counterclockwise (CCW) direction (see figure).

 

 

The above interpretation is based on a depth perception caused by binocular ‘disparity’ (binocular depth-from-disparity). It is traditionally what we considered with the vocabulary ‘stereopsis’.

 

The Pulfrich pendulum also contains motion cues. After the image of one eye is delayed, at any moment, the pendulum image on two retinas has different motion vectors. The brain fuses the pendulum images of two eyes, and its ‘motion-in-depth’ can be calculated from these two motion vectors. This is motion stereopsis, a depth perception generated by the difference of binocular motion vectors (binocular depth-from-motion).

 

Besides disparity cue, the depth perception of the Pulfrich pendulum also contains motion cue.

 

Clinically, we have found patients without disparity stereopsis, such as those with small-angle esotropia, can still recognize whether the Pulfrich pendulum is rotating clockwise or counterclockwise. Their depth perception must come from binocular motion cue (binocular depth-from-motion), because disparity cue (binocular depth-from-disparity) is absent in their brain.

 

 

The following link is a Pulfrich pendulum test program (only executable on Windows system, cannot run on Mac or Android).

Pulfrich.exe

 

This is a program that simulates the Pulfrich pendulum. There are two pendulums in red and blue colors respectively. Wearing red/blue goggles, blue lens over right eye and red lens over left eye. Only one pendulum is seen by each eye.

 

The sinewave moving pendulum goes along a 360° cycle in 36 steps, 10° for each step. There is a time difference between the pendulum of the left eye and the pendulum of the right eye. The time difference between the two eyes is set by the program to be 1 step to 15 steps. The less the time difference, the less the delay between the left and right eyes of the simulated Pulfrich pendulum, thus the less obvious the depth perception, and it is then less easier to see it as an elliptical orbit; The more the time difference, the more the delay between the left and right eyes of the simulated Pulfrich pendulum, thus the more obvious the depth perception, and it is then easier to see as an elliptical orbit. If the left eye (red) is delayed, it will be seen as a clockwise rotation; if the right eye is delayed, it will be seen as a counterclockwise rotation.

 

The left eye delay or right eye delay, i.e. the clockwise or counterclockwise rotation of the track, is randomly selected by the computer.

The subject’s work is to determine whether the elliptical track closer to him/her is moving leftward or rightward. The subject input the answer by hitting <left> or <right> key on the keyboard. If the answer is correct, computer gives a crescendo tone, and if the answer is incorrect, there will be a decrescendo tone.

 

If you can't judge the rotation direction and always give wrong answer, you can increase the delay between the left and right eyes by hitting <+> key, that will increase the depth of the elliptical track to make it more obvious and easier to answer. At the beginning of the program, the default delay is 3 steps.

Pressing the <+> key will increase the delay by 1 step, making the depth judgment easier; pressing the <-> key will decrease the delay by 1 step, making depth judgment more difficult. The program sets a minimum delay of 1 step and a maximum delay up to 15 steps.

 

The subjects increase or decrease the delay number by him/herself, to determine the threshold where they can correctly tell the CW/CCW direction.

 

By pressing <Enter> key, you can see the current delay number in the upper-left corner of the screen. This delay serves as a threshold of depth judgement of the Pulfrich pendulum.

 

To stop the program, press <Esc> key.

 

The title page of the program is shown in the picture

 

 

This is an example of counterclockwise (CCW) rotation.

 

This is an example of clockwise (CW) rotation.